Cognitive Neuroscience Society

The Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

  • Facebook
  • RSS
  • Twitter
MENUMENU
  • Home
  • Annual Meeting
        • General Information
          • What to Expect at CNS 2023
          • CNS 2023 Mobile App
          • CNS 2023 Giveaway
          • CNS 2023 Giveaway Winners
          • Accessibility at CNS
          • General Information
          • Code of Conduct
          • Dates and Deadlines
          • Hotel Reservations
          • Poster Guidelines
          • Poster Printing Discount
          • Annual Meeting Workshop Policy & Application
          • Exhibit with us!
        • Program
          • Thank you to our Partners
          • CNS 2023 Program Booklet
          • Schedule Overview
          • Program-at-a-Glance
          • CNS 30th Anniversary Dance Party
          • Keynote Address
          • George A. Miller Awardee
          • Distinguished Career Contributions Awardee
          • Young Investigator Awardees
          • CNS at 30: Perspectives on the Roots, Present, and Future of Cognitive Neuroscience
          • Invited-Symposium Sessions
          • Symposium Sessions
          • Data Blitz Session Schedule
          • Poster Schedule & Session Information
          • JoCN Travel Fellowship Award
          • GSA/PFA Award Winners
          • Workshops, Socials & Special Events
        • Registration
          • Registration
          • Registration FAQ
          • Registration Policies, Cancellations & Refunds
        • News/Press
          • CNS 2023 Press Room
          • CNS 2022 Blog
          • CNS 2021 Blog
          • CNS 2020 Blog
        • Submissions
          • 2023 Poster Printing Discount
          • Submission Requirements
          • Submit a Poster
          • Submit a Symposium
          • GSA or PFA Application
          • Data Blitz
          • Frequently Asked Submission Questions
        • Archive
          • CNS 2020 Conference Videos
          • CNS 2019 Conference Videos
          • CNS 2018 Conference Videos
          • CNS 2017 Conference Videos
          • CNS 2016 Conference Videos
          • CNS 2015 Conference Videos
          • Previous Meetings Programs & Abstracts
  • About CNS
    • Boards and Committees
    • CNS Statement: Black Lives Matter
  • Membership
    • Information and Benefits
    • Join or Renew Membership
    • Membership FAQs
    • Member Discounts
    • Newsletter
      • Submit an Announcement
      • Current Newsletter
      • Newsletter FAQs
      • Past Newsletters
  • Awards
    • George A. Miller Award
    • Fred Kavli Distinguished Career Contributions Award
    • Young Investigator Award
    • Young Investigator Award Nominations
    • 2023 YIA Nomination Form
    • JoCN Travel Fellowship Award
  • News Center
    • CNS Blog
    • CNS 2023 Press Room
    • CNS 2022 Blog
    • CNS 2021 Blog
    • CNS 2020 Blog
    • CNS 2019 Blog
    • Blog Archives
    • Quick Tips for Getting Started on Twitter
    • Media Contact
  • My CNS
  • Contact Us
post

Time for Understanding Time in the Brain

March 19, 2019

CNS 2019

“Time is passing too fast!” Many of us use that phrase every day when we feel like our kids are growing up fast or when a deadline sneaks up on us. When Virginie van Wassenhove hears that phrase, it conjures an entirely different point of view. She goes straight to consciousness, musing on how we perceive reality.

“When it comes to time, we tend to use linguistic shortcuts that may abuse the state of reality and fundamentally bias the way we think about time and the way scientists conceptualize issues related to time,” she says. “I am interested in understanding how the slow time scales of squishy matter afford us to assign meaning to reality.”

A cognitive neuroscientist at CEA and INSERM in Paris, van Wassenhove is working to understand the neural underpinnings of time. She has organized a symposium on the topic at the Cognitive Neuroscience Society (CNS) annual meeting in San Francisco this month — featuring scientists who are exploring evidence of how we construct mental models of time.

“Our work suggests that the conscious arrow of time — thinking about the past or the future — is calibrated on where we imagine ourselves to be, what the brain represents to be the ‘here and now.'” -Virginie van Wassenhove

Traditionally, she explains, research on time has focused on how the brain quantifies duration and responds to external rhythms. Now, comparisons of mental time and space at the computational level are offering promising venues to explore new questions on how we perceive time in the brain — questions, van Wassenhove says, like “do high-level representations of time, unlike those of space, show an obligatory orientation past to future?”

“Our work suggests that the conscious arrow of time — thinking about the past or the future — is calibrated on where we imagine ourselves to be, what the brain represents to be the ‘here and now,’” she says. Van Wassenhove’s team’s work thus suggests that how we sequence time is determined by internal representations of ourselves.

For Marc Howard, a cognitive neuroscientist at Boston University, his fascination with time dates back to childhood. “The brain’s estimate of the past and the future is foundational to our experience,” he says. “We don’t experience a series of isolated moments. We experience a stream of memories receding gradually towards the past and a stream of predictions approaching from the future.” Our memory is thus a timeline that we must construct, he says.

What is exciting about the field now, Howard says, is that scientists are finding neurophysiological evidence to support what was once only theoretical mathematical models. “Over the last several years we’ve seen growing evidence that there’s a record of time in various parts of the brain,” Howards says. “These ‘time cells’ behave like we would expect from a compressed record of the past and have been observed in many brain regions, e.g. hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, and the striatum.”  

“The field of neuroscience will have to further mature and embrace the fact that it will not be possible to understand the human mind without describing how the brain tells, represents, and conceptualizes time.” -Dean Buonomano

In even newer work he’ll be discussing at the CNS meeting, researchers have now found evidence to support a mathematical construct of time called a “Laplace transform.” Last summer, Albert Tsao in the Moser lab at the Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience and Centre for Neural Computation, which first discovered time cells, published a paper showing evidence that suggested the lateral entorhinal cortex maintains the Laplace transform of time.

Howard described the Laplace transform as a fun-house mirror reflection of an image that shows the pattern of events in time that lead to the present. “The reflection doesn’t look much like the image you started with,” he says. “But different images would give different reflections.  If you knew the mapping between each image and its reflection, you could look at a reflection and guess what image caused it.”

For several years, Howard and colleagues have argued that the brain constructs time as a Laplace transform and a corresponding inverse. (The inverse in the funhouse analogy, he says, is the shape of the funhouse mirror.)  In this construct, time cells estimate the past but with a systematic distortion predicted by the Laplace equations; the estimate gets more blurry for events further in the past.

Howard’s lab is working to replicate the Tsao data in monkeys to show that the Laplace transform equations govern this construct. “If this result holds up across tasks and species, it suggests that the equations are telling us something relatively deep about the way the brain estimates time,” he says. “It suggests we can work out a real theory of memory and maybe cognition more broadly.”

Indeed, Howard’s ultimate goal is to create “simple, elegant, and powerful descriptions of cognition.” While it’s still early, he says, “these new results convince me that this goal is at least possible.”

Indeed, van Wassenhove quotes Dean Buonomano (Your Brain is a Time Machine, 2018): “The field of neuroscience will have to further mature and embrace the fact that it will not be possible to understand the human mind without describing how the brain tells, represents, and conceptualizes time.”

-Lisa M.P. Munoz

By lmunoz Filed Under: featured Tagged With: cns 2019, memory, time Leave a Comment

Previous article: Putting Together the Puzzle of Adaptive Constructive Memory
Next article: CNS 2019 Day 1 in Brief

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

You must be logged in to post a comment.

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.

Recent Posts

  • Poverty: What’s the Brain Got to Do With It?
  • Unraveling Graceful Human Learning Over Time
  • Looking Forward to Understand Working Memory
  • From the Neurology Clinic to the Lab and Back Again: Addressing Frontal Lobe Syndromes
  • When Philosophical Questions Turn to Neuroscience Experimentation

Blog Archives

Quick Tips for Getting Started on Twitter

Cognitive Neuroscience Society
c/o Center for Mind and Brain
267 Cousteau Place, Davis, CA 95618
916-955-6080: for CNS Membership Questions
805-450-7490: for annual meeting questions about- registration, posters, symposium
916-409-5069: Fax Line
email: meeting@cogneurosociety.org

Recent Posts

  • Poverty: What’s the Brain Got to Do With It?
  • Unraveling Graceful Human Learning Over Time
  • Looking Forward to Understand Working Memory
  • From the Neurology Clinic to the Lab and Back Again: Addressing Frontal Lobe Syndromes
  • When Philosophical Questions Turn to Neuroscience Experimentation

Archives

Blog Archives

Previous Meeting Programs and Abstracts

Past Newsletters

All contents © Cognitive Neuroscience Society 1995-2019

Add to Calendar

Add to Calendar
04/16/2022 11:00 AM
04/16/2022 12:00 PM
America/Los_Angeles
How Prior Knowledge Shapes Encoding of New Memories
Description of the event
Grand Ballroom A
Create an Account

Login Utility