Cognitive Neuroscience Society

The Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

  • Facebook
  • RSS
  • Twitter
MENUMENU
  • Home
  • Annual Meeting
        • General Information
          • What to Expect at CNS 2023
          • CNS 2023 Mobile App
          • CNS 2023 Giveaway
          • CNS 2023 Giveaway Winners
          • Accessibility at CNS
          • General Information
          • Code of Conduct
          • Dates and Deadlines
          • Hotel Reservations
          • Poster Guidelines
          • Poster Printing Discount
          • Annual Meeting Workshop Policy & Application
          • Exhibit with us!
        • Program
          • Thank you to our Partners
          • CNS 2023 Program Booklet
          • Schedule Overview
          • Program-at-a-Glance
          • CNS 30th Anniversary Dance Party
          • Keynote Address
          • George A. Miller Awardee
          • Distinguished Career Contributions Awardee
          • Young Investigator Awardees
          • CNS at 30: Perspectives on the Roots, Present, and Future of Cognitive Neuroscience
          • Invited-Symposium Sessions
          • Symposium Sessions
          • Data Blitz Session Schedule
          • Poster Schedule & Session Information
          • JoCN Travel Fellowship Award
          • GSA/PFA Award Winners
          • Workshops, Socials & Special Events
        • Registration
          • Registration
          • Registration FAQ
          • Registration Policies, Cancellations & Refunds
        • News/Press
          • CNS 2023 Press Room
          • CNS 2022 Blog
          • CNS 2021 Blog
          • CNS 2020 Blog
        • Submissions
          • 2023 Poster Printing Discount
          • Submission Requirements
          • Submit a Poster
          • Submit a Symposium
          • GSA or PFA Application
          • Data Blitz
          • Frequently Asked Submission Questions
        • Archive
          • CNS 2020 Conference Videos
          • CNS 2019 Conference Videos
          • CNS 2018 Conference Videos
          • CNS 2017 Conference Videos
          • CNS 2016 Conference Videos
          • CNS 2015 Conference Videos
          • Previous Meetings Programs & Abstracts
  • About CNS
    • Boards and Committees
    • CNS Statement: Black Lives Matter
  • Membership
    • Information and Benefits
    • Join or Renew Membership
    • Membership FAQs
    • Member Discounts
    • Newsletter
      • Submit an Announcement
      • Current Newsletter
      • Newsletter FAQs
      • Past Newsletters
  • Awards
    • George A. Miller Award
    • Fred Kavli Distinguished Career Contributions Award
    • Young Investigator Award
    • Young Investigator Award Nominations
    • 2023 YIA Nomination Form
    • JoCN Travel Fellowship Award
  • News Center
    • CNS Blog
    • CNS 2023 Press Room
    • CNS 2023 Blog
    • CNS 2022 Blog
    • CNS 2021 Blog
    • CNS 2020 Blog
    • CNS 2019 Blog
    • Blog Archives
    • Quick Tips for Getting Started on Twitter
    • Media Contact
  • My CNS
  • Contact Us
post

Enhancing the Sleeping Brain

October 17, 2017

Guest Post by Sadie (Sarah) Witkowski, Northwestern University

As one of five children, my mom has plenty of stories about her and her siblings’ misadventures. One of my favorites revolves around my “weird” Uncle Dorsey and his early scientific endeavors. When my mom was about 8 years old, her older brother slipped a tape player under her bed every night to quietly play a reading of “The Raven” by Edgar Allen Poe. Night after night he would play the tape, trying to test whether my mom would spontaneously recite the poem from all her exposure. The way she tells it, my mom woke up every time the recording started to play. Sure, she can still recite the first few lines, but only because she was awakened by the poem night after night.

My uncle never managed to get my mom to “sleep learn” (aka hypnopedia) but it turns out some of his ideas might not have been so misguided. While sleep learning has been debunked, cognitive neuroscientists in the Paller Lab at Northwestern University where I work, and others are now discovering ways to use stimuli like sound or odor cues during sleep to strengthen memories.

Early researchers made the same mistake as Uncle Dorsey and thought we could learn new material during sleep, ala Brave New World. As in Aldous Huxley’s dystopian novel, experiments attempting this sleep learning were often based on the false premise that sleep is just like a hypnotic state. In the 1920s, some researchers believed they could teach people totally new information by playing audio recordings to a sleeping participant. Inventors got in on the racket, selling devices that promised to teach you as you slept. It wasn’t much different from the episode of Dexter’s Lab when he tries to use a massive gadget to teach himself French as he sleeps. But unlike in the cheesy cartoon, these devices didn’t work during sleep.

Not until the 1950s did researchers discover the touted effects of hypnopedia were actually not due to sleep at all. Instead these contraptions were actually awakening people. The debunkers could tell by using a relatively established technique called electroencephalography (EEG), recording the brain’s electrical signals through electrodes placed on the scalp. Using EEG on sleep learning participants, researchers could tell that participants were actually awake (something we still do in research today). This revelation all but ended research regarding using sleep as a cognitive tool. Fifty years later, we neuroscientists now know it is possible to alter memory during sleep, just in a different way than previously expected.

In a 2007 paper, neuroscientists at Lübeck University reported that smells, which were associated with previously learned material, could be used to cue the sleeping brain. The study authors had taught participants the locations of objects on a grid, just like the game concentration, and exposed them to the odor of roses as they did so. Next, participants slept in the lab and the experimenters waited until the deepest stage of sleep, slow-wave sleep, to once again expose them to the odor. When they awoke, the participants were significantly better at remembering where the objects were located. This only worked if they had been exposed to the rose smell at learning and then smelled the odor during slow-wave sleep. If they were only given the odor during sleep or during REM sleep, the cue did not work.

It almost seemed too good to be true. Could scientists really “tag” memories during learning with a smell and then persuade the participant’s brain to rehearse it during sleep? Multiple studies since the initial paper verified the finding, and there were even new variations such as using sound cues instead of odors.

The Paller Lab has since published multiple papers demonstrating that neuroscientists can link a single sound to a single object and reactivate it individually. For example, as a study participant, you could play concentration and learn the cat is in the lower left corner and the teakettle is in the upper right. When you learned the cat, you heard a meow, and when you learned the kettle, you heard a whistle. Now if we were to play only one of those sounds during slow-wave sleep such as the meow, you would actually remember the cat’s location even better than the kettle. Keep in mind, both of these items were initially learned equally well, showing the sleep-cueing preferentially helped the cat. This ability to select specific memories to reactivate is called targeted memory reactivation (TMR).

We call it TMR because we believe playing a sound cue like this reactivates the memory of learning the object’s location from the previous task. Those at my lab believe this replay of the memory allows the brain to strengthen its memory representation, thus leading to better recall. Working with rats, Daniel Bendor and Matthew Wilson of MIT found exactly this expected replay when they administered sound cues related to previous learning.

Neuroscientists are now starting to put this TMR to work. One recent study from my group operated a lot like Guitar Hero. James Antony, now a postdoc at Princeton University, but a graduate student at the time, had participants learn two musical sequences on a keyboard. The “songs” were composed of four “notes,” which appeared as falling circles on the screen, much like the actual video game. After learning both songs equally well, participants took a nap during which Antony cued them with one of the songs. When they were retested after sleep, participants were better at the sleep-cued song than the uncued one. Think of how quickly you could learn a new musical instrument or song, just by reminding your sleeping brain of previous learning!

We’re still on the frontier of understanding the sleeping brain. For an activity that we do for about one-third of our life, there are more questions than answers.

Although some translational research has started to surface, we still don’t know the boundaries of TMR or sleep cueing in general. In a 2017 paper, postdoctoral researcher Laura Batterink, also in the Paller Lab, found that TMR cueing in combination with REM sleep led to better recall of vocabulary words that were cued during an afternoon nap. This research seems to point to REM sleep as a beneficial state when cued memories are integrated into preexisting memory networks.

Outstanding questions that we have yet to address include: Does this work for foreign language (i.e., grammar learning) learning, or just learning foreign vocabulary words? Could it be used to help maintain memory performance in an aging population? Does reactivating some memories mean that others are wiped away even more quickly?

I’m personally interested in how these reactivated memories might be changing due to these cues. My current projects are aimed at investigating whether cueing causes the whole memory to maintain detail or whether these cues might be consolidating the ‘gist’ of the memory, thereby losing extraneous detail. Alternatively, TMR may help boost all aspects of memory consolidation and show cognitive costs only for uncued items.

We have some hunches, and new projects to address these questions are cropping up all the time. But we’re still on the frontier of understanding the sleeping brain. For an activity that we do for about one-third of our life, there are more questions than answers. Maybe weird Uncle Dorsey and his Poe tapes actually were prescient of the future for sleep research.

–

Sadie Witkowski grew up in the small town of Dripping Springs, Texas, before attending University of Texas at Austin for her dual degrees in Psychology and Plan II Honors. From there, she moved to Chicago and is currently working on her Ph.D. in psychology at Northwestern University, focusing on sleep and memory. In her spare time, Witkowski interviews graduate students about their research for her podcast, PhDrinking.

Are you a member of CNS with research you want to share? Consider contributing a guest post about your work or trends in the field. It could then become featured on the online magazine Aeon. Email your ideas to CNS Public Information Officer, Lisa M.P. Munoz (cns.publicaffairs[@]gmail.com). 

By lmunoz Filed Under: featured Tagged With: learning, sleep, sound

Previous article: The Effects of Stress on Learning Vary by Memory Type
Next article: Unwinding the Movie Reels in the Mind’s Eye

Recent Posts

  • Psychedelics and Cognition: A New Look
  • CNS 2023: Day 3 Highlights
  • CNS 2023: Day 2 Highlights
  • Forget About It: Investigating How We Purge Thoughts from Our Minds
  • CNS 2023: Day 1 Highlights

Blog Archives

Quick Tips for Getting Started on Twitter

Cognitive Neuroscience Society
c/o Center for Mind and Brain
267 Cousteau Place, Davis, CA 95618
916-955-6080: for CNS Membership Questions
805-450-7490: for annual meeting questions about- registration, posters, symposium
916-409-5069: Fax Line
email: meeting@cogneurosociety.org

Recent Posts

  • Psychedelics and Cognition: A New Look
  • CNS 2023: Day 3 Highlights
  • CNS 2023: Day 2 Highlights
  • Forget About It: Investigating How We Purge Thoughts from Our Minds
  • CNS 2023: Day 1 Highlights

Archives

Blog Archives

Previous Meeting Programs and Abstracts

Past Newsletters

All contents © Cognitive Neuroscience Society 1995-2019

Add to Calendar

Add to Calendar
04/16/2022 11:00 AM
04/16/2022 12:00 PM
America/Los_Angeles
How Prior Knowledge Shapes Encoding of New Memories
Description of the event
Grand Ballroom A
Create an Account

Login Utility